When Mick Jagger first sang “What a drag it is getting old,” he was 23
years old. Now at 69, he is still a veritable Jumpin' Jack Flash on
stage. Jagger seems to have found the secret to staying physically fit
in his advancing years, but getting old can be a drag on the psyche.
Many older adults fear memory loss and worry they are headed down the
road to dementia, such as Alzheimer's disease. Every time they forget
their keys, leave a door unlocked or fail to remember a name, they are
reminded of this nagging concern. In most cases, however, such annoying
incidents are part of normal age-related memory loss, not a sign of
impending dementia.
Although lots of older adults think such a decline is inevitable, there
is good news for many of them. Researchers have developed an array of
helpful methods and activities that exercise our minds and bodies that
can help keep the older mind in relatively good condition. In this
column, we examine the most promising ways to shore up memory in the
normal aging brain.
Memory Divided
Memory is not a single entity. The term encompasses several types of
remembering, not all of which decline with age. For instance, older
people still retain their vocabulary, along with general knowledge about
the world (semantic memory). They can also perform certain routine
tasks, such as making an omelet or typing on a computer (procedural
memory), about as well as they could when they were younger. People do
become worse, however, at recalling recent events in their lives
(episodic memory) or where they first learned a piece of information
(source memory), managing the temporary storage of short-term
information (working memory), and remembering to do things in the future
(prospective memory).
Prospective memory, in particular, is an important target for memory
strategies because forgetting upcoming tasks or appointments can cause
considerable frustration or embarrassment. In 2002 psychologist Narinder
Kapur of Southampton General Hospital in England and his colleagues
reviewed studies on the effectiveness of various common techniques to
bolster prospective memory. They found that external aids such as making
lists or programming reminders into a cellphone could be helpful in
reducing memory problems such as failing to pay bills or attend
meetings.
Another successful strategy involves associating information to be
recalled with an image, sentence, phrase or word. The more personally
relevant the association is, the more likely it is to be remembered, an
approach known as self-referential processing. For example, if we need
to return a book to the library, we might imagine ourselves doing just
that. Made-up acronyms also can be a big help. In this strategy, a
person forms a new word from the initial letters of what he or she wants
to remember. To remember to buy a birthday gift for his wife, for
example, a man might construct the acronym “BIG” for “Buy Gift.”
In 2008 psychologists Betty L. Glisky of the University of Arizona and
Martha L. Glisky of the Evergreen Hospital Medical Center described
other useful methods for improving memory that involve visual or
semantic elaboration. In one of these, a person conjures up images
related to something he or she wants to retain. To remember the name
“Peggy,” you might imagine a pirate with a wooden (peg) leg. Such a
tactic could be helpful as long as you do not end up calling her
“Pegleggy.”
A semantic approach entails tacking on words to what you wish to recall.
For example, in a music appreciation class that one of us (Arkowitz)
took in elementary school, the teacher asked the class to associate the
main musical theme of the classical piece, the Peer Gynt Suite,
with the following rather silly sentence: “Morning is dawning and Peer
Gynt is yawning and music is written by Grieg.” The tie-in with the
phrase was designed to help the kids remember the name of the composer.
Although Glisky and Glisky found support for these visual and semantic
techniques, among others, they cautioned that memory improvements in the
laboratory do not necessarily translate to enhancements in daily life,
because these benefits depend on people practicing and using the tactics
regularly. This gap in efficacy may be widest for strategies that take
considerable time and effort to learn. Also, improvements in one area of
memory often do not generalize to others.
Studies have found some support for the validity of the saying “Use it
or lose it.” The more we use our memory—for example, reading, doing
crossword puzzles and playing board games—the better it may be, probably
because such activities involve considerable use of memory. Of course,
those with better memories may also be more likely to exercise their
minds in the first place, accounting for some (but probably not all) of
the association between good memory and amount of cognitive stimulation.
Fit Body, Fit Mind
If Jagger is as physically fit as he looks, his mind may be following
suit. Some studies have found that higher levels of aerobic exercise are
associated with better memory in older adults. Although many of these
studies do not prove that aerobic exercise causes the memory
improvements, some do suggest a causal connection. When psychologist
Stanley Colcombe of the University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign and
his colleagues reviewed 18 controlled studies addressing this
association in 2003, they found evidence that aerobic exercise did
indeed lead to enhancements in memory.
Sustained aerobic activity may not be the only way to keep your mind
agile and your memory sharp. In a study published in 2011 neurologist
Ruth Ruscheweyh of the University of Münster in Germany and her
colleagues assessed total physical activity in 62 older adults over six
months. Their questionnaire included both formal exercise and daily
routines such as walking to work, climbing stairs and gardening. The
researchers linked reported increases in overall activity, no matter its
type, with improvements in episodic memory at the end of six months.
The greater the rise in activity levels, the bigger the memory boost.
Thus, keeping physically active through regular workouts along with
everyday errands and tasks may be the best recipe for reinvigorating
your powers of recollection. [For more on the connection between
physical and mental fitness in old age, see “Fit Body, Fit Mind?” by
Christopher Hertzog, Arthur F. Kramer, Robert S. Wilson and Ulman
Lindenberger; Scientific American Mind, July/August 2009.]
The research suggests that many memory techniques as well as a
physically and mentally energetic lifestyle can improve memory in older
adults. We still have a long way to go before we have highly effective
methods, but given the vigor of this field, we can expect great progress
in the near future.
Tomado de: http://www.scientificamerican.com/article.cfm?id=memory-in-old-age-can-be-bolstered&page=2
No hay comentarios:
Publicar un comentario